Journal Information

Article Information


Street harassment perception and its relations with self-objectification of women

 

Abstract

Between 7 and 36% of women around the world report having been victimized through a violent act (UN 2006). Street harassment is an implicit type of violence that deprives women’s freedom of movement. There are no statistics of this type of violence in the Dominican Republic, but generally, cultural practice encourages both men and women to accept it. This study seeks to relate the perception of street harassment, self-objectification in women and their reactions to harassment. We used a non-probabilistic convenience sampling of 46 Dominican women working in a private university in Santo Domingo. Results indicate that the acceptance of sexual harassment has a low positive correlation with objectification, and a high positive correlation with the acceptance of myth of domestic violence. A lower acceptance of street harassment is related to higher levels of education and social status. A greater effort to prevent street harassment is associated with lower self-objectification of women.

Resumen

Entre el 7 y el 36% de las mujeres en todo el mundo informan haber sido víctimas de un acto violento (ONU 2006). El acoso callejero es un tipo implícito de violencia que priva a las mujeres de su libertad de movimiento. No existen estadísticas de este tipo de violencia en la República Dominicana, pero, en general, la práctica cultural alienta a hombres y mujeres a aceptarla. Este estudio busca relacionar la percepción de acoso callejero, autobjetivación en las mujeres y sus reacciones al acoso. Utilizamos un muestreo de conveniencia no probabilístico de 46 mujeres dominicanas que trabajan en una universidad privada en Santo Domingo. Los resultados indican que la aceptación del acoso sexual tiene una baja correlación positiva con la objetivación y una alta correlación positiva con la aceptación del mito de la violencia doméstica. Una menor aceptación del acoso callejero se relaciona con niveles más altos de educación y condición social. Un mayor esfuerzo para prevenir el acoso callejero se asocia con una menor autobjetivación de las mujeres.


Violence against women occurs frequently around the world, between 7 and 36% of women reported having experienced sexual abuse or some form of violence (UN 2006). At the same time 59% of women report have been victims of sexual aggression by her intimate partner before age 15. Street harassment is a type of violence against women that deprives them of accessing essential basics needs, sociocultural events, enjoyment, and impacts their mental and physical health. One type of sexual harassment that is frequently trivialized is sexual harassment in the streets (Bowman 1993).

The United Nations Entity for Gender Equality and the Empowerment of Women (UNW 2014) explains that street harassment is a type of violence that restricts the victim’s movement, reducing their ability to attend school and work. Stop Street Harassment (SSH 2015) defines street harassment as an encounter between the perpetrator and the victimized person, in which the first one performs unwanted comments, gestures and forced actions without consent. Such actions are performed in terms of the ways the perpetrator perceives the gender, sex, gender expression or sexual orientation of the victim.

Street harassment represents a form of violence against the victimized person and may lead to significant experiences of psychological distress (Ekore 2012). Street harassment usually occurs between a man and a woman, the man being the perpetrator and the victim a woman (Candidate 2015). Bowman (1993) states that the potential victim of street harassment is any woman who is sexually developed, but not too old or beyond reproductive age, because stereotypically, an older woman is not considered a beauty standard. Street harassment is considered as “sexual terrorism”, a type of systematic control and domination that men use against women through implicit violence (Kissling 1991).

Sexual terrorism and gender-based violence play a fundamental role in the subordination of women (Ayres, Friedman & Leaper, 2009). This type of violence usually provokes in the victimized person from mild discomfort to intense fear (Bowman 1993). Several studies report that street harassment generates feelings of terror for women and reinforces their fear of being raped and other acts of terrorism (Davis 1994). McMillan, Nierobisz & Welsh (2000) mentioned that street harassment has a negative impact on women and increases their fear to be in public places, which makes them reduce their sense of security.

While for many women street harassment is an intrusion into personal space, a humiliating experience, a potential threat a type of violence, and abuse against them (Kissling 1991; Packer 2008; Ekore 2012) that women and men differ in what constitutes street harassment. These individual differences underscore the importance of studying this type of gender base violence in the context of the cultural and societal structures that perpetuate male violence against women. The origins of violence against women are based on a sociocultural construction of gender, and power. Studies show that street harassment is one of the most common types of violence against women in the world (Bowman 1993; Fredrickson & Roberts 1997). The victim’s response to street harassment is a very important component and can be particularly susceptible to the influences of culture (Wasti & Cortina 2002).

A typical response to street harassment is to ignore the perpetrator. The less frequent coping mechanism against street harassment is to report or face the perpetrator (Fairchild & Rudman 2008). When women have an active response against street harassment, the perpetrator usually criticizes, shouts and can possibly physically attack the woman. Women who create a habit of constantly ignoring the perpetrator can be considered as passive victims. It has been found that perpetrators that are ignored tend to feel free to repeat the harassment (Bowman 1993). When women often have an active reaction, it may be dangerous for them, because the perpetrator may react aggressively, leading women to be at the crossroads to have an active or passive reaction towards the harassment. This type of reaction can be difficult to accept for men, because they are part of the group of power and the culture in a society that is dominated by men. Although men can perceive street harassment cases as isolated and rare, for women it is an experience of sexual terrorism and violence against them that is chronic in nature and can lead to burnout (Candidate 2005).

Street harassment is a problem that occurs in many countries worldwide. For example, the SSH (2014) conducted a national survey that interviewed 2,000 participants with the aim of studying the prevalence of street harassment in the United States. This study found that 65% of women surveyed have experienced some kind of street harassment. Within this group, 23% have been sexually touched, 20% have been followed, and 9% have been forced to perform a sexual act such as oral sex or masturbation without their consent. Most of the time the perpetrator is a man, but both men and women can be victimized by street harassment. In the same study, 25% of men who have been harassed are part of the LGBTQI community and the kind of harassment they experienced were homophobic and transphobic insults.

McMillan et al. (2000) made a study with a sample of 12,300 Canadian women to research the impact of street harassment and their perception of their sense of security. The results indicated that 80% of women experienced street harassment by an unknown man, and that affected the way they perceived their safety on the streets. The UNW (2015) together with the European Union conducted a study of street harassment in Chile. The results indicated that 85% of the Chilean women have experienced some kind of street harassment. Also, Vallejo & Rivarola (2013) developed a study in Peru where they interviewed 800 women, the results showed that 60% of women have experienced street harassment and over 80% were between 18 and 29 years old. In general, these reports indicate that women are at a considerable risk of being victimized by men, and street harassment is the type of violence they use against women. Although street harassment may vary with race, economic status, or ethnicity of the victimized person, as well as the history of interactions between genders that she might had have, women tend to be victims of other type of violence such as sexism and objectification (Bowman 1993).

Sexism and gender stereotypes

Glick & Fiske (1996) explain that sexism is a multidimensional construct that incorporates not only hostility towards women, but two attitudes sets such as sexists: benevolent sexism and hostile sexism. Benevolent sexism are the stereotypical and specific attitudes towards women according to the gender roles that society imposes, whereas hostile sexism are, the type of prejudices, hostile affections, and negative stereotypes towards women (Glick & Fiske 1996). When men and women are exposed to benevolent and sexist stereotypes, only women get high levels of self-monitoring, body shame, and appearance management.

These stereotypes remind women their subordination to men, positioning women as things and objects that need care and protection of men (Calogero 2013). Some argue that street harassment and other coercive behavior are part of larger dimension of hostility towards women (Ayres et al. 2009; Bowman1993; Kissling 1991). Accordingly, although sexist hostility plays an important role in the perception of street harassment, ambivalent sexism is a predictor of tolerance towards it.

Sexism is linked to the root of culturally stereotypical traditional gender roles; there are reasons to believe that those who adopt traditional gender roles are more tolerant of street harassment (Rusell & Trigg 2004). Gender stereotypes imposed by society to women could affect their perception and reaction to street harassment because stereotypes are enforced according to men’s perspective and interactions. These stereotypes affect men’s perception so they can’t understand women’s needs and don’t respect their rights (Davis 1994). In modern society the gender stereotypes imposed on women are as sex objects, while for men are as sex predators. These stereotypes are promoted through television, movies, advertising and magazines, particularly in media’s focus on teens (Jewell & Spears 2013). Media and communications manage sexual objectification through visual presentation of bodies and content that highlights the importance of appearance (Aubrey 2006). Studies, additionally, indicate that the media often portray a limited and often elusive standard of women’s physical beauty and link this standard with women’s sexual identity and worth. When women have exposure to sexually objectifying media they can give more importance to beauty and appearance to define their individual’s own self-worth (Szymanski, Moffitt & Carr 2011). Grabe, Shibley & Ward (2008) affirm that repeated exposure to media content leads viewers to begin to accept media portrayals as representation of reality. Consuming sexually objectifying and self-objectifying television, movies advertising and magazines can also increase self-objectification on girls and women. Culture affects media representation of girls and women through stereotypes and values them as sex objects. These stereotypes can possible limit women’s self-perception and can impact their notion regarding the importance of appearance (Gordon 2008). Street harassment is based on the cultural domination of women, although women and men share the same culture, women are subordinate because the culture is sexist (Davis 1994).

Objectification and self-objectification

Fredrickson & Roberts (1997) affirm that objectification occurs when women or any part of their bodies, or some sexual functions are separated from them, reducing them only to the state of instruments or things. In the other hand self-objectification occurs when people appraises their own bodies from the perspective of a third person, focusing only on the observables attributes of the body rather than the perspective of the first person, which focuses on non-observables privileges and attributes (Noll & Fredrickson 1998).

Several studies have shown that women who adopt the point of view of self-objectification, is because they have lived in a society that legitimizes sexist ideologies and gender roles in a culturally chauvinistic society (Calogero 2013; Fairchild & Rudman 2008). This is why street harassment can trigger both physical and psychological damage to a woman, because when a man makes an inappropriate comment or stares at a woman’s body part it reinforces the objectification of women. Thus women dehumanize themselves and they learn through their experiences to associate emotions of humiliation and worthlessness as part of their sexual identity (Candidate 2005).

Davidson, Gervais & Shred (2013) conducted a study with 495 college women where they measured the impact related to street harassment and the objectification and self-objectification. The study reveals that while less known is the perpetrator to the victimized person, more likely the victim self-objectify. On the other hand, the authors explain that self-objectification has a ripple effect that leads not only to victim self-objectification, but also objectify others, therefor affects women’s world vision.

Socioeconomic status and sexual harassment

Kearl (2014) ruled a study which indicated that people of color, low income, and people who self-identified as lesbians, gay, bisexuals, or trans were disproportionally affected by street harassment. According to (Fredrickson & Roberts 1997), women of the aforementioned groups face an additional negative position as racism, classism, homophobia and transphobia. Women develop a coping mechanism where looks are more important to them so men can treat them better. These strategies are not necessarily conscious or deliberate. Society external pressure through subtle exposure of street harassment makes them seek to improve their physical beauty voluntarily or even naturally. This makes them self-objectify themselves, and makes them have less perception of street harassment and more acceptance of it (Bowman 1993).

Gender based violence in the Dominican Republic

In the last 10 years, Dominican Republic has experienced one of the highest rates of gender-based violence compared to previous years. In 2015, 93 women were killed while 61 were killed in 2014. Existing data state that 6,608 gender based violence cases has been reported in 2015 (Procuraduría General de la República 2015). However, national statistics systems aren’t trustworthy, characterized by reports absence, and conflicting information can be found. There’s anecdotal evidence provided by non-profit organizations (NGOs) that claim that gender-based violence’s figures are higher than reported (Oficina Nacional de Estadistica 2014). In the Dominican Republic have been carried out several studies about sexual harassment without regard of street harassment. The Ministerio de la Mujer de la República Dominicana (2010) prepared a study on sexual harassment and moral harassment at work, where they conducted surveys in 19 public institutions and 38 private companies located in Santo Domingo. The results indicate that 31.9% of the women working in the public sector have experienced sexual harassment, and 31.4% of the women working in the private sector were victimized by sexual harassment in the Dominican Republic. The difference between the groups was not significant.

Legislation on sexual harassment

There’s no law in the Dominican Republic that penalizes street harassment, but there’s an article in the Labor Code that penalizes sexual and moral harassment. The Article 49.9 of the Labor Code of the Dominican Republic explains that the employer cannot take any action that is considered sexual harassment, as well as intervene or support in the case of his representatives. Despite the aforementioned, there’s no sanction, which means that the inspectors cannot take any act against the employers. On the other hand, the penal code promulgated Law 2497 on Interfamily Violence, which includes sexual harassment, this law defines sexual harassment in its article 333-2 as the order, threat, obtain favors of a sexual nature, performed by a person who abuses his duties; this infraction entails a year of imprisonment and from RD$5,000 to RD$10,000 of fines (Ministerio de la Mujer 2010).

According to Bowman (1993), there are few studies on street harassment because there are few legal remedies in this regard; it is almost impossible to legally subject a stranger to street harassment because it disappears immediately, and it is unlikely that lawmakers will support any law against such violence. The United Nations Human Rights (2015) passed a Law 30314, which explains that sexual harassment in public places should be prevented and punished. It also explains that street harassment is a type of violence against women, and as all public and private entities must adhere to this Law. In the United States, all states have laws that penalize harassment, but not all specifically target street harassment, for example, in the state of Hawaii implemented Harassment Act 711-1106, which explains that if a man continues talking to a person in the streets even though a woman tells him to stop; if a person insults, harasses and uses offensive words, or puts offensive physical contact to other person; can be reported to the authorities (Hawaii State Legislature 2006).

The first country in Latin America to create a law against street harassment was Peru. This law explains the responsibilities of its public institutions to prevent and punish such violence. It mentions that it is the obligation of the Ministry of Education to create a curriculum for teaching and prevention of it, at the same time, mentions that the Ministry of Transport and Communications must issue notices against street harassment on public transport. It mentions that this law does not have penal sanctions, but fines (Congreso de la República de Perú 2015).

While it is true that governments in different parts of the world are aware that street harassment involves sexual terrorism in the form of implicit oppression of women, and that they are taking various actions in this regard. In many cases, the victim does not realize that he or she is a victim of street harassment, as it is culturally an “accepted” fact. The studies and surveys conducted are biased, if the alleged victim does not find out that she has been violated. Likewise, it is important to know if the victim, even though she knows she is a victim, accepts harassment as part of “being a woman”.

Objectives and hypotheses

As street harassment is a form of implicit violence that develops through cultures that are based on gender stereotypes, hostile and benevolent sexism, it mainly affects women and girls. They learn that the female body is a sexual object and were created to please men; in this way they learn to self-objectified and objectified others. The present study seeks to establish the relationship between perceived sexual harassment on the street, the self-objectification of women and their reaction to harassment. Specifically, we propose the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1: The greater acceptance of street harassment, the greater the self-objectification.

Hypothesis 2: The greater the socioeconomic and educational level of women, the less their acceptance of street harassment.

Hypothesis 3: We hope that a greater effort to avoid street harassment is related to a decrease in self-objectification.

Method

Participants

This study was carried out with women who work both in the cleaning area and in the administrative area at a private university in Santo Domingo. Based on a non-probabilistic sampling for convenience, 46 women with a range between 19 and 54 years old (M = 33.15, S = 9.13) participate voluntarily in this research. All the participants were of Dominican nationality, being 56.5% single, 26.1% in free union and 17.4% married. 65.2% said they had children compared to 30.4% who said they did not have children, 4.3% did not answered the question. Regarding the educational level of the participants, 21.7% reached the middle school, 23.9% high school, 39.1% college, a 8.7% specialty or master degree and 6.5% did not respond. Finally, the level of income reported was higher for incomes between RD$6,000 and RD$12,000 with 41.3%, then those with incomes higher than RD$50,000, with a 17.4%, followed by 13.0% for incomes of RD$12,000 to RD$20,000, 10.9% for income between RD$20,000 and RD$30,000, 8.7% for income of less than RD$6,000 and 8.7% for income between RD$30,000 and RD$50,000.

Instruments

Participants were given a sociodemographic questionnaire that assessed aspects such as age, marital status, and number of children, educational level and income level. In addition, the participants completed a battery of instruments composed of the following scales:

  • Domestic violence myth acceptance scale (DVMAS), developed by Peters (2006) and translated into Spanish by the author of this research, was used to measure the acceptance of domestic violence myths. The scale has 18 items divided into 4 sub-scales called personality fault (7 items), behavioral fault (5 items), exoneration (3 items) and minimization (3 items). The full scale features an Alpha Cronbach of .81, while the subscales range from .64 to .88.

  • Objectified body consciousness scale (OBCS), developed by McKinley (1996) and translated into Spanish by the author, was used to measured self-objectification. It consists of 24 items divided into 3 sub-scales of 8 items: vigilance, shame towards the body and belief control; with reliabilities of .89, .75 and .72, respectively. The full scale has a reliability of .93.

  • Sexual harassment was measured using the Sexual experiences questionnaire (SEQ), developed by Fitzgerald (1995) and translated into Spanish by the author. It has 23 items divided into 4 sub-scales: sexist hostility (4 items), sexual hostility (8 items), unwanted sexual attention (6 items) and sexual coercion (5 items), with reliabilities of .83, .91, .85 and .95, respectively.

In all scales the items were answered based on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from Not agree (1) to Completely agree (5). In addition, scales were elaborated by the author to measure the constructs of efforts to avoid unwanted sexual experiences (9 items), acceptance of street harassment (31 items), victimization of street harassment (12 items) and frequency of harassment sexual orientation (21 items). The items of these scales were elaborated from a focal group of 8 people, who suggested indicators of street harassment.

Table 1 presents the descriptive results and skills for the instruments used, both the total scales and their sub-scales. The least reliable scales are those of sexist hostility (( = .49), considered as unacceptable; and minimization (( = .53), considered to be poor. The other scales are in ranges ranging from questionable to good (( = .61 to (= .89). Among the most reliable scales are the acceptance of street harassment (( = .89) and frequency of street harassment (( = .89), developed for the purposes of this research.

Table 1

Descriptive statistics and reliability for used scales.

Scale Min. Max. Mean DT ɑ
S1. Age 19 54 33.15 9.131 -
B1. Personal guilt 1 5 3.19 .807 .61
B2. Behevioral guilt 2 5 2.59 .847 .78
B3. Exoneration 1 5 3.26 1.199 .76
B4. Minimization 1 5 2.97 .999 .53
BT. DVM acceptance 2 5 3.00 .773 .85
C1. Sexist hostility 1 5 3.11 1.025 .49
C2. Sexual hostility 1 5 3.24 1.032 .82
C3. USA 1 5 3.33 1.181 .69
C4. Sexual coercion 1 4 1.86 .787 .62
CT. Sexual experiences 1 4 2.88 .786 .87
DT. EAUSE 1 5 2.42 .715 .67
ET. SH acceptance 1 4 2.32 .643 .89
FT. Victimization against SH 2 4 2.65 .624 .84
GT. SH frequency 2 4 2.48 .511 .89
H1. Body shame 1 4 2.09 .704 .64
H2. Feeling control 1 5 3.20 .876 .75
H3. Self-monitoring 1 5 3.02 .817 .81
HT. Self-objectification 1 4 2.77 .627 .80

[i] Note. N = 46; USA: unwanted sexual atention; EAUSE: efforts to avoid unwanted sexual experiences; SH: Street Harassment; DVM: domestic violence myths.

[ii] Source: Self elaboration.

Procedures

The educational center where the study was conducted was chosen for convenience. After having the approval of the ethics committee of the institution to carry out the research, the battery was developed to be used. In the first instance, the offices responsible for personnel involved in the investigation were contacted, then contacted directly and obtained their informed consent, which guarantees the confidentiality of their information, as well as the voluntary nature of their participation. They could withdraw at any time without any consequence. Finally, the participants completed the battery, which was numbered and stored to maintain confidentiality. Due to the type of information that the battery handles, the participants were given a flyer with information on how to get therapeutic help in case they needed it.

The managed battery information was tabulated and stored for analysis in SPSS version 20 database (for Mac). After obtaining the descriptive results, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient (() was used to calculate the reliability of the scales, which was interpreted according to the guide provided by George and Mallery (2003), which suggests that (≥ .90 can be considered as excellent, ( ≥ .80 as good, ( ≥ .70 as acceptable, (≥ .60 as questionable, ( ≥ .50 as poor, and (< .50 as unacceptable. Finally, Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) was used to evaluate the degree of relationship between the different variables. According to Cohen (1998), values of | r | ≥ .50 indicate high correlations, | r | ≥ .30 median correlations, and, | r | ≥ .10 low correlations.

Results

Correlations between variables

Table 2 shows the correlations between some sociodemographic variables and the totals in the scales used. First, with regard to hypothesis 1 that higher street harassment (SH) is associated with greater self-objectification, the results indicate a low positive correlation between SH acceptance and self-objectification (r = .29, p < .05). On the other hand, SH acceptance shows a high positive correlation with the acceptance of myths of domestic violence (r = .58, p < .01) and with age (r= .52, p < .01).

Table 2

Correlations between sociodemographic variables and total scales.

S1 S2 S3 BT CT DT ET FT GT HT
S1. Age 1

S2. Education level -.57** 1

S3. Socioeconomic level -.32* .65** 1

BT. DVM acceptance .23 -.51** -.56** 1

CT. Sexual expiriences -.04 -.27 -.04 -.01 1

DT. EAUSE -.10 .28 .33* -.16 -.06 1

ET. SH acceptance .52** -.51** -.39** .58** -.03 .01 1

FT. SH victimization -.28 .46** .32* -.29* -.15 .48** -.06 1

GT. SH frequency .10 -.24 -.13 .37* .42** .16 .19 -.12 1

HT. Self-objectification .15 -.36* -.28 .43** .10 -.48** .29* -.23 .24 1

[i] Note. N=46; **p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; DVM: domestic violence myths; EAUSE: efforts to avoid unwanted sexual experiences; SH: street harassment.

[ii] Source: Self elaboration.

The results also support hypothesis 2, which states that a lower acceptance of SH is related to a higher educational and economic level, indicating a high negative correlation with the educational level (r = -.51, p < .01) and negative with the socioeconomic level (r = -39, p < .01). Likewise, the educational level and the economic level correlated negatively with the acceptance of domestic violence myths (r = -.51, p < .01 and r = -.56, p < .01, respectively). In addition, victimization of SH correlated positively with the educational and socioeconomic level (r = .46, p < .01 and r = .32, p < .05, respectively).

Referring to hypothesis 3, which argued that a greater effort to avoid street harassment would be related to lower self-objectification, the results show a mean negative relationship between the effort to variable to avoid unwanted sexual experiences and self-objectification (EAUSE; R = -.48, p < .01), as well as a mean positive correlation with SH victimization (r = .48, p < .01). The last variable also showed a low negative correlation with DVM acceptance (r = -.29, p < .05).

Interestingly, the age variable showed a high negative correlation with educational level (r = -.57, p < .01) and mean negative correlation with socioeconomic status (r = -.32, p < .05). Likewise, it shows a high positive correlation with SH acceptance (r = .52, p < .01).

Table 3 shows the correlations between the sub-scales of the instruments used. First, the control feeling sub-scale, belonging to the self-objectification scale, shows a high positive correlation with the personal guilt sub-scales (r =.53, p < .01) and behavioral guilt (r = .53 p, < .01); (r = .39, p < .01) and minimization (r = .34, p < .05), belonging to the scale of acceptance DVM. Also, the self- monitoring sub-scale, on the self-objectification scale, shows mean positive correlations with personal guilt scales (r = .41, p < .01) and behavioral guilt (r =.43, p < .01). However, it should be noted that the body-shame scale did not show significant correlations with any of the other sub-scales.

Table 3

Correlations between scales and subscales used.

  B1 B2 B3 B4 BT C2 C3 CT DT ET FT GT H2 H3 HT
B1. Personal guilt 1                            
B2. Behavioral guilt .65** 1                          
B3. Exoneration .52** .51** 1                        
B4. Minimization .45** .56** .49** 1                      
BT. Acceptance of DVM .78** .82** .82** .78** 1                    
C2. Sexual hostility .18 -.01 -.14 .28 .08 1                  
C3. USA .07 -.03 -.02 .17 .06 .61** 1                
CT. Sexual experiences .05 -.12 -.14 .20 -.01 .85** .82** 1              
DT. EAUSE -.13 -.25 -.04 -.13 -.16 -.17 .17 -.06 1            
ET. SH acceptance .47** .35* .62** .35* .58** -.04 -.04 -.03 .01 1          
FT. SH victimization -.42** -.32* -.05 -.24 -.29* -.19 -.04 -.15 .48** -.06 1        
GT. SH frequency .35* .23 .19 .42** .37* .51** .48** .42** .16 .19 -.12 1      
H2. Control feeling .53** .53** .39** .34* .54** .21 .03 .00 -.30* .34* -.25 .28 1    
H3. Self-monitoring .41** .43** .24 .29 .41** .40** .01 .18 -.61** .10 -.38** .23 .60** 1  
HT. Self-objectification .45** .43** .29 .25 .43** .30* .02 .10 -.46** .29* -.23 .24 .82** .86** 1

[i] Note. N = 46; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; DVM: domestic violence myths; USA: unwanted sexual attention; EAUSE: efforts to avoid unwanted sexual experiences; SH: street harassment; sub-scales C1 (Sexual hostility), C4 (Sexual coercion) y H1 (Body shame) eliminated because they did not presented significant correlations with the other scales.

[ii] Source: Self elaboration.

The relationship between SH acceptance and self-objectification is specifically linked to the control feeling sub-scales, showing a mean positive correlation with it (r = .34, p < .05). The acceptance of SH showed a high positive correlation with the exoneration sub-scale (r = .62, p < .01) and mean positive with the minimization sub-scales (r = .35, p < .05), personal guilt (r = .47, p < .01) and behavioral fault (r = .35, p < .05), belonging to the DVM acceptance scale. On the other hand, the SH frequency shows a high positive correlation with sexual hostility (USA; r = .51, p < .01) and positive mean with unwanted sexual attention sub-scale (r = .48, p < .01), belonging to the scale of sexual experiences.

Finally, efforts to avoid unwanted sexual experiences show a mean positive correlation with SH victimization scale (r = 48, p < .01), a mean negative correlation with the control feeling sub-scale (r = -.30, p < .05) and high negative with the self-monitoring sub-scales (r = -.61, p < .01), belonging to the self-objectification scale.

Discussion

In a culture that legitimizes sexist ideologies and gender stereotypes towards women (Calogero 2013), street harassment becomes a type of violence that has proved to be limiting to women adequate performance in the society (UNW 2014). Self-objectification is a component that underlies this type of culture where the female body is sexually objectified (Fredicson & Roberts 1997). The main objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between the perception of street harassment, the self-objectification of women and the reaction to street harassment. In this sense, the general findings of this research suggest that the greater the acceptance of street harassment, the greater the self-objectification of women. At the same time, there is a relationship between the acceptance of violence’s myths and the acceptance of street harassment. On the other hand, according to (Karl 2014), the people who most accept street harassment are people of low socioeconomic status.

The results indicate that women with higher socioeconomic and educational level scored less acceptance of street harassment and myths of violence. However, these women tend to victimize more. It was also shown that, while women make more effort to avoid unwanted sexual experiences, they have fewer possibilities to self-objectify. In spite of this, these women also tend to have greater victimization. It was shown that women who make more effort to avoid unwanted sexual experiences tend to have less acceptance of the domestic violence myths. An interesting fact that should be highlighted in the results of this research is that older women tend to have a greater acceptance of street harassment. In the same order, it was observed that the more sense of control over their bodies, the more they tended to experience more personal guilt and more behavioral guilt which also related to the exoneration of the perpetrator and the minimization of domestic violence myths. Also, it was shown that the more women self-monitor their bodies, the more personal and behavioral guilt they experience. It is important to mention that when a woman is exposed more frequently to street harassment she is more vulnerable to sexual hostility and unwanted sexual attention. This agrees with (Gilck & Fisk1996), that sexual hostility adversely affects the perception of women, making them more tolerant of street harassment. Finally, it was shown that women who make an effort to avoid unwanted sexual experiences tend to become more victimized by street harassment and monitor their bodies less. The above results agree with the international literature and explains that street harassment is an implicit type of violence that is part of the continuity of violence and sexism against women which is linked to gender stereotypes imposed by society. This study showed that gender stereotypes, and the power group (men) play an important role in the tolerance of street harassment (Rusell & Trigg 2004).

Limitations

The following research is not without limitations. First, the size of the sample was small to be representative of the population of Santo Domingo, according to the international literature it is more likely that street harassment occurs in large cities (Bowman 1993). On the other hand, the short time during which the research was carried out prevented the elaboration of a parallel social experiment as planned. The lack of studies on the subject limited the creation of a standardized instrument and had to join several pre-existing ones, where the researcher had to make an instrument to measure specific and contextualized street harassment.

Recommendations

It is recommended to conduct a research at a national level that takes into account street harassment and to use a statistically representative sample to be aware of this type of violence that affects the majority of women and girls in the world as expressed in the literature and this study demonstrates. With scientific research like this one, what is intended is to change public policies in favor of the general population, we also recommend that efforts be initiated to work on a bill that penalizes street harassment so the culture stops allowing it as natural behavior. In addition, an instrument that can measure street harassment in the Dominican Republic should be created and standaraized in order to improve citizen security and prevent this type of violence. Ending street harassment requires an effort by all, not only as individuals, but also an effort as a community and as a society. To make these changes we must involve boys and men by educating community leaders, stat leaders, lawyers, entrepreneurs and ministries of education so that we can create safer places.

References

1 

Aubrey, Jennifer Stevens. «Exposure to sexually objectifying media and body self-perceptions among college women: An examination of the selective exposure hypothesis and the role of moderating variables.» Sex Roles 55, no. 3-4 (2006): 159-172.

Jennifer Stevens Aubrey Exposure to sexually objectifying media and body self-perceptions among college women: An examination of the selective exposure hypothesis and the role of moderating variablesSex Roles3-42006159172

2 

Ayres, Melanie, Carly Friedman & Leaper Campbell. «Individual and situational factors related to young women’s likelihood of confronting sexism in their everyday lives.» Sex Roles 29th (April): 449-460, 2009.

Melanie Ayres Carly Friedman Leaper Campbell Individual and situational factors related to young women’s likelihood of confronting sexism in their everyday livesSex Roles4494602009

3 

Biblioteca del Congreso Nacional de Chile. Regulando el acoso sexual callejero en America Latina. www.bcn.cl (Consulted, May 13th, 2016).

Biblioteca del Congreso Nacional de Chile. Regulando el acoso sexual callejero en America Latina www.bcn.cl May 13th, 2016

4 

Bowman, Cynthia. «Street harassment and the informal ghettoization of women.» Harvard Law Review 3rd (January): 517-580, 1993.

Cynthia Bowman Street harassment and the informal ghettoization of womenHarvard Law Review5175801993

5 

Calogero, Rachel. «Objects don’t object: evidence that self-objectification disrupts women’s social activism.» Psychological Science 22d (January): 312-318, 2013.

Rachel Calogero Objects don’t object: evidence that self-objectification disrupts women’s social activismPsychological Science3123182013

6 

Candidate, J. «Stresmut: Gender, media, and the legal power dynamics of street harassment, or “Hey Sexy” and other verbal ejaculations.» Columbia Journal of Gender and Law. 2005. eds.b.ebscohost.com (Consulted, 16th September, 2015).

J. Candidate Stresmut: Gender, media, and the legal power dynamics of street harassment, or “Hey Sexy” and other verbal ejaculationsColumbia Journal of Gender and Law2005 eds.b.ebscohost.com 16th September, 2015

7 

Laniya, Olatokunbo Olukemi. «Street smut: gender, media, and the legal power dynamics of street harassment, or hey sexy and other verbal ejaculations.» Colum. J. Gender & L. 14 (2005): 91.

Olatokunbo Olukemi Laniya Street smut: gender, media, and the legal power dynamics of street harassment, or hey sexy and other verbal ejaculationsColum. J. Gender & L.1420059191

8 

Cohen, Jacob. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, rev. ed. New York: Taylor & Francis Inc., 1977.

Jacob Cohen Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciencesNew YorkTaylor & Francis Inc.1977

9 

Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences , 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Hillsddale, NJ. 1998.

J. Cohen Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences2Hillsdale, NJ, EnglandLawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.1998

10 

Congreso de la República de Perú. Ley para Prevenir y Sancionar el Acoso Sexual en Lugares Públicos. www.congreso.gob.pe (Consulted, February 11th, 2016).

Congreso de la República de Perú Ley para Prevenir y Sancionar el Acoso Sexual en Lugares Públicos www.congreso.gob.pe February 11th, 2016

11 

Davidson, S., Gervais, S., Sherd, L. «The ripple effects of stranger harassment on objectification of self and others.» Psychology of Women Quarterly, Vol. 20. 2013. eds.b.ebscohost.com (Consulted, 28th September, 2015).

S. Davidson S. Gervais L. Sherd The ripple effects of stranger harassment on objectification of self and othersPsychology of Women Quarterly202013 eds.b.ebscohost.com 28th September, 2015

12 

Davidson, M. Meghan, Sarah J. Gervais and Lindsey W. Sherd. «The ripple effects of stranger harassment on objectification of self and others.» Psychology of Women Quarterly 39, no. 1 (2015): 53-66.

M. Meghan Davidson Sarah J. Gervais Lindsey W. Sherd The ripple effects of stranger harassment on objectification of self and othersPsychology of Women Quarterly39120155366

13 

Davis, Deirdre. «The harm that has no name: Street harassment, embodiment, and African American women.» Ucla Women’s LJ 4 (1993): 133.

Deirdre Davis The harm that has no name: Street harassment, embodiment, and African American womenUcla Women’s LJ41993133133

14 

Davis, D. (1994). «The harm that has no name: Street harassment embodiment, and African American women.» UCLA Women’s Law Journal, Vol. 4. 1994. eds.b.ebscohost.com (Consulted, 8th June, 2016).

D. Davis 1994The harm that has no name: Street harassment embodiment, and African American womenUCLA Women’s Law Journal4 eds.b.ebscohost.com 8th June, 2016

15 

Ekore, John O. «Gender differences in perception of sexual harassment among university students.» Gender and Behaviour 10, no. 1 (2012): 4358-4369.

John O. Ekore Gender differences in perception of sexual harassment among university studentsGender and Behaviour101201243584369

16 

Fairchild, Kimberly and Laurie A. Rudman. «Everyday stranger harassment and women’s objectification.» Social Justice Research 21, no. 3 (2008): 338-357.

Kimberly Fairchild Laurie A. Rudman Everyday stranger harassment and women’s objectificationSocial Justice Research2132008338357

17 

Fitzgerald, Louise F., Michele J. Gelfand and Fritz Drasgow. «Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances.» Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17, no. 4 (1995): 425-445.

Louise F. Fitzgerald Michele J. Gelfand Fritz Drasgow Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advancesBasic and Applied Social Psychology1741995425445

18 

Fredrickson, Barbara L. and Tomi-Ann Roberts. «Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks.» Psychology of Women Quarterly 21, no. 2 (1997): 173-206.

Barbara L. Fredrickson Tomi-Ann Roberts Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risksPsychology of Women Quarterly2121997173206

19 

George, Darren and M. Mallery. Using SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference. 2003.

Darren George M. Mallery Using SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference2003

20 

Glick, Peter and Susan T. Fiske. «The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism.» Journal of personality and social psychology 70, no. 3 (1996): 491.

Peter Glick Susan T. Fiske The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexismJournal of personality and social psychology7031996491491

21 

Gordon, Maya K. «Media contributions to African American girls’ focus on beauty and appearance: Exploring the consequences of sexual objectification.» Psychology of Women Quarterly 32, no. 3 (2008): 245-256.

Maya K. Gordon Media contributions to African American girls’ focus on beauty and appearance: Exploring the consequences of sexual objectificationPsychology of Women Quarterly3232008245256

22 

Grabe, Shelly, L. Monique Ward and Janet Shibley Hyde. «The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies.» Psychological bulletin 134, no. 3 (2008): 460.

Shelly Grabe L. Monique Ward Janet Shibley Hyde The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studiesPsychological bulletin13432008460460

23 

Honorable Cámara de Diputados de la Nación, República de Argentina. Ley para la Prevención y Sanción del Acoso Sexual en Espacios Públicos. www.hcdn.gob.ar (Consulted, February 11th, 2016).

Honorable Cámara de Diputados de la Nación, República de Argentina Ley para la Prevención y Sanción del Acoso Sexual en Espacios Públicos www.hcdn.gob.ar February 11th, 2016

24 

Jewell, Jennifer A. and Christia Spears Brown. «Sexting, catcalls, and butt slaps: How gender stereotypes and perceived group norms predict sexualized behavior.» Sex Roles 69, no. 11-12 (2013): 594-604.

Jennifer A. Jewell Christia Spears Brown Sexting, catcalls, and butt slaps: How gender stereotypes and perceived group norms predict sexualized behaviorSex Roles6911-122013594604

25 

Harassment, Stop Street. «Unsafe and harassed in public spaces: A national street harassment report.» Stop street harassment, 2014.

Harassment, Stop Street Unsafe and harassed in public spaces: A national street harassment reportStop street harassment2014

26 

Kearl, H. (2014). Unsafe and harassed in public spaces: A national street harassment report. 2014. www.stopstreetharassment.org (Consulted, 16th October, 2015).

H. Kearl 2014Unsafe and harassed in public spaces: A national street harassment report www.stopstreetharassment.org 16th October, 2015

27 

Kissling, Elizabeth Arveda. «Street harassment: The language of sexual terrorism.» Discourse & Society 2, no. 4 (1991): 451-460.

Elizabeth Arveda Kissling Street harassment: The language of sexual terrorismDiscourse & Society241991451460

28 

Legislatura del Estado de Hawaii. Harassment Law. www.capitol.hawaii.gov (Consulted, february 11th, 2016).

Legislatura del Estado de Hawaii Harassment Law www.capitol.hawaii.gov february 11th, 2016

29 

Macmillan, Ross, Annette Nierobisz and Sandy Welsh. «Experiencing the streets: Harassment and perceptions of safety among women.» Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 37, no. 3 (2000): 306-322.

Ross Macmillan Annette Nierobisz Sandy Welsh Experiencing the streets: Harassment and perceptions of safety among womenJournal of Research in Crime and Delinquency3732000306322

30 

Ministerio de la Mujer República Dominicana. Investigación sobre el acoso sexual y el acoso moral en el trabajo. www.mujer.gob.do (Consulted, February 8th, 2016).

Ministerio de la Mujer República Dominicana Investigación sobre el acoso sexual y el acoso moral en el trabajo www.mujer.gob.do February 8th, 2016

31 

McKinley, Nita Mary andJanet Shibley Hyde . «The objectified body consciousness scale development and validation.» Psychology of Women Quarterly 20, no. 2 (1996): 181-215.

Nita Mary McKinley Janet Shibley Hyde The objectified body consciousness scale development and validationPsychology of Women Quarterly2021996181215

32 

Noll, Stephanie M. and Barbara L. Fredrickson. «A meditational model linking self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eating.» Psychology of Women Quarterly 22, no. 4 (1998): 623-636.

Stephanie M. Noll Barbara L. Fredrickson A meditational model linking self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eatingPsychology of Women Quarterly2241998623636

33 

Oficina Nacional de Estadística. Violencia contra la mujer perpetrada por la pareja o por la ex pareja en la República Dominicana: Situación actual y estado de la formación. www.one.gob.do (Consulted, June 15th, 2015).

Oficina Nacional de Estadística Violencia contra la mujer perpetrada por la pareja o por la ex pareja en la República Dominicana: Situación actual y estado de la formación www.one.gob.do June 15th, 2015

34 

OUNW. Acoso sexual en lugares públicos. www.endvawnow.org (Consulted, February 11th, 2016).

OUNW Acoso sexual en lugares públicos www.endvawnow.org February 11th, 2016

35 

Packer, Jaclyn. «Sex differences in the perception of street harassment.» Women & Therapy, 5, no. 2-3 (2008): 331-338. 2008. ds.a.ebscohost.com (Consulted, February 1rst, 2016).

Jaclyn Packer Sex differences in the perception of street harassmentWomen & Therapy52008331338 ds.a.ebscohost.com February 1rst, 2016

36 

Peters, Jay. «Measuring myths about domestic violence: Development and initial validation of the domestic violence myth acceptance scale.» Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma 16, no. 1 (2008): 1-21.

Jay Peters Measuring myths about domestic violence: Development and initial validation of the domestic violence myth acceptance scaleJournal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma1612008121

37 

Procuraduría General de la República. Reportes de violencia doméstica. www.pgr.gob.do (Consulted, December 22nd, 2015).

Procuraduría General de la República Reportes de violencia doméstica www.pgr.gob.do December 22nd, 2015

38 

Russell, Brenda L. and Kristin Y. Trigg. «Tolerance of sexual harassment: An examination of gender differences, ambivalent sexism, social dominance, and gender roles.» Sex Roles 50, no. 7 (2004): 565-573.

Brenda L. Russell Kristin Y. Trigg Tolerance of sexual harassment: An examination of gender differences, ambivalent sexism, social dominance, and gender rolesSex Roles5072004565573

39 

SSH. What is street harassment. 2014. www.stopstreetharassment.org (Consulted, 5th May, 2015).

SSH What is street harassment2014 www.stopstreetharassment.org 5th May, 2015

40 

Szymanski, Dawn M., Lauren B. Moffitt and Erika R. Carr. «Sexual objectification of women: Advances to theory and research 17.» The Counseling Psychologist 39, no. 1 (2011): 6-38.

Dawn M. Szymanski Lauren B. Moffitt Erika R. Carr Sexual objectification of women: Advances to theory and research 17The Counseling Psychologist3912011638

41 

UNW. Creating safe public spaces. 2014. www.unw.org (Consulted, May 15th, 2015).

UNW Creating safe public spaces2014 www.unw.org May 15th, 2015

42 

UN. Poner fin a la violencia contra la mujer. De las palabras a los hechos. 2016. www.un.org (Consulted, February 11th, 2016).

UN Poner fin a la violencia contra la mujer. De las palabras a los hechos2016 www.un.org February 11th, 2016

43 

Vallejo, Elizabeth and María Paula Rivarola. La violencia invisible: acoso sexual callejero en Lima Metropolitana y Callao. Perú: Pontificia Universidad Católica de Perú, Cuadernos de Investigación IOP, 2013.

Elizabeth Vallejo María Paula Rivarola La violencia invisible: acoso sexual callejero en Lima Metropolitana y CallaoPerúPontificia Universidad Católica de PerúCuadernos de Investigación IOP2013

44 

Wasti, S. Arzu and Lilia M. Cortina. «Coping in context: Sociocultural determinants of responses to sexual harrassment.» Journal of personality and social psychology 83, no. 2 (2002): 394.

S. Arzu Wasti Lilia M. Cortina Coping in context: Sociocultural determinants of responses to sexual harrassmentJournal of personality and social psychology8322002394394