An experimental study of coperation in a natural seting

Main Article Content

Juan Botella
Óscar García Leal
Laura Moreno
Shih Pei-Chun
José Santacreu

Abstract

During the last several decades research in social behavior has allowed to differentiate at least two different types of interactions: cooperation and competition. Specifically, several attempts have been made to predict and explain cooperative behavior. Typically, it has been studied using artificial situations (e.g. Azrin & Lindsley, 1956; Cohen & Lindsley, 1964; Lindsley, 1966; Mithaug & Burgess, 1967, 1968; Schmitt, 1987; Schmitt & Marwell, 1968; Shimoff & Matthews, 1975), being the most typical matrix games like the Prisoner’s Dilemma, but recent studies have incorporated more naturalistic situations. Following the research initiated by Ribes-Iñesta (Ribes-Iñesta, 2001; Ribes- Iñesta & Rangel, 2002) we show how a computerized puzzle-solving task can be used to improve our knowledge of dyadic interactions, as minimal settings representative of social behavior. In three studies, the candidates for a job position could cooperate or not cooperate with another candidate by helping with the other’s puzzles. Results show that the behaviors could be classified in three groups: non-cooperation, graded cooperation, and systematic cooperation. These behavioral tendencies were highly consistent throughout the task and reasonably stable after a one-year interval. Their distribution is not independent of gender; females show a higher frequency of non-cooperative behavior than of systematic cooperation, whereas males show the reverse. These results are in accordance with recent reports in the literature (e.g. Kurzban & Houser, 2001). As previous studies, we demonstrate that the tendency to cooperate is influenced by the cooperative tendency of the others.

Article Details

How to Cite
Botella, J., García Leal, Óscar, Moreno, L., Pei-Chun, S., & Santacreu, J. (2011). An experimental study of coperation in a natural seting. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 31(2), 261–281. https://doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v31.i2.23250